Despite Their Acute Sense of Hearing, Dogs May Never Learn That Every Sound of a Word Matters

Dog With EEG

Despite their excellent auditory capacities, dogs do not attend to differences between words which differ only in one speech sound (e.g. dog vs dig), according to a new study by Hungarian researchers of the MTA-ELTE ‘Lendület’ Neuroethology of Communication Research Group at the Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest (ELTE) measuring brain activity with non-invasive electroencephalography (EEG) on awake dogs. This might be a reason why the number of words dogs learn to recognize typically remains very low throughout their life. The study is published in Royal Society Open Science. Credit: Vivien Reicher

Despite their excellent auditory capacities, dogs do not attend to differences between words which differ only in one speech sound (e.g. dog vs dig), according to a new study by Hungarian researchers of the MTA-ELTE ‘Lendület’ Neuroethology of Communication Research Group at the Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest (ELTE) measuring brain activity with non-invasive electroencephalography (EEG) on awake dogs. This might be a reason why the number of words dogs learn to recognize typically remains very low throughout their life. The study is published in Royal Society Open Science.

Dogs can distinguish human speech sounds (e.g. “d,” “o” and “g”) and there are similarities in the neuronal processing of words between dogs and humans. However, most dogs can only learn a few words throughout their lives, even if they live in a human family and are surrounded by human speech. Magyari and her colleagues hypothesized that despite dog’s human-like auditory capacities for analyzing speech sounds, dogs might be less ready to attend to all differences between speech sounds when they listen to words.

Dog Under EEG Test

Electroencephalography (EEG) is an often-used technique in human clinical and research studies and it has been also successfully applied on tranquilized, sleeping, or awake but trained dogs. However, in this study, the researchers measured EEG on awake dogs without any specific training. Credit: Elodie Ferrando

To test this idea, the researchers developed a procedure for measuring electrical activity in the brain non-invasively on awake, untrained, family dogs. Electroencephalography (EEG) is an often-used technique in human clinical and research studies and it has been also successfully applied on tranquilized, sleeping, or awake but trained dogs. However, in this study, the researchers measured EEG on awake dogs without any specific training.

Researchers invited dogs and their owners to the lab. After the dog got familiar with the room and the experimenters, the experimenters asked the owner to sit down on a mattress together with her dog as it would be a relaxation time for them. Then, the experimenters put electrodes on the dog’s head and fixed it with a tape. With the electrodes on, dogs listened to tape-recorded instruction words they knew (e.g. “sit”), to similar but nonsense words (e.g. “sut”), and to very different nonsense words (e.g. “bep”).

“The electroencephalography is a sensitive method not only to brain activity but also to muscle-movements. Therefore, we had to make sure that dogs tense their muscles as little as possible during measurement. We also wanted to include any type of family dogs in our study not only specially trained animals. Therefore, we decided that instead of training our dog participants, we would ask them just to relax. Of course, some of the dogs who came to the experiment could not settle down and did not let us do the measurement. However the dropout rate from the study was similar to the dropout rate in EEG studies with human infants. It was also an exciting process for us to learn how we can create a relaxing and safe atmosphere in the lab for both the dogs and their owners,” says lead author Lilla Magyari, postdoctoral researcher at Department of Ethology, Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary.

The analysis of the recorded electric brain activity showed that dog brains clearly and quickly discriminated the known words from the very different nonsense words starting from 200 ms after the beginning of the words. This effect is in line with similar studies on humans which show that the human brain responds differently to meaningful and nonsense words already within a few hundred milliseconds. But the dogs’ brains made no difference between known words and those nonsense words that differed in a single speech sound only. This pattern is more similar to the results of experiments with human infants who are around 14 months old. Infants become efficient in processing phonetic details of words, which is an important prerequisite for developing a large vocabulary, between 14 and 20 months. But younger infants do not process phonetic details of words in certain experimental and word learning situations despite the fact that infants are able to differentiate speech sounds perceptually within weeks after birth.

“Similarly to the case of human infants, we speculate that the similarity of dogs’ brain activity for instruction words they know and for similar nonsense words reflects not perceptual constraints but attentional and processing biases. Dogs might not attend to all details of speech sounds when they listen to words. Further research could reveal whether this could be a reason that incapacitates dogs from acquiring a sizable vocabulary,” concludes Attila Andics, principal investigator of the MTA-ELTE ‘Lendület’ Neuroethology of Communication Research Group.

Reference: “Event-related potentials reveal limited readiness to access phonetic details during word processing in dogs” by Lilla Magyari, Zsófia Huszár, Andrea Turzó and Attila Andics, 9 December 2020, Royal Society Open Science.
DOI: 10.1098/rsos.200851

This study was published on the 9th of December in Royal Society Open Science titled “Event-related potentials reveal limited readiness to access phonetic details during word processing in dogs,” written by Lilla Magyari, Zsófia Huszár, Andrea Turzó and Attila Andics. This research was funded by the Hungarian Academy of Sciences (Lendület Program), the National Research, Development and Innovation Office and the Eötvös Loránd University (ELTE).

4 Comments on "Despite Their Acute Sense of Hearing, Dogs May Never Learn That Every Sound of a Word Matters"

  1. Michael Whipple | December 10, 2020 at 5:08 pm | Reply

    I totally disagree!…my last lab “zeb” knew exactly what certain words meant…he would not eat until i told him the words “o” “k”….he would sit awaiting his permission to eat…I could say “o” “z”, “o” “b” even “o” “t ay” and he would not eat until he heard the “k” sound…
    Best dog I’ve ever had…miss him terribly

  2. Michael, they don’t use Labs in these experiments. They’re too intelligent.

  3. Stand in front of your dog and just mouth the word “sit”. You may find that your dog follows the command as if you said the word out loud. Dogs may not need to differentiate similar sounding words because they use a combination of sound and (even very subtle) visual cues to understand us.

  4. This is not my experience. I don’t think this study is wrong, just stopped short. Just like commenter Michael, I’ve also messed with my dogs by throwing them off during tricks and feeding with similar but non-exact phonetics. Yes at first a they will react to a similar sound, but with training they surely are capable knowing the difference, no matter how slight, and react accordingly. Humans are like this too when learning a new language. Think how difficult it is to tune yourself into specific phonetics, especially when it’s not part of your first language, and especially especially trying to manage the different accents a new language can have.

Leave a Reply to jacob S Cancel reply

Email address is optional. If provided, your email will not be published or shared.