Science’s New Replication Crisis: Research That Is Less Likely to Be True Is Cited More

Confused Scientist

Papers in prominent psychology, economic, and science journals that fail to replicate, and consequently are less likely to be accurate, often become the most cited papers in academic research.

Papers that cannot be replicated are cited 153 times more because their findings are interesting, according to a new UC San Diego study.

Papers in leading psychology, economic, and science journals that fail to replicate and therefore are less likely to be true are often the most cited papers in academic research, according to a new study by the University of California San Diego’s Rady School of Management.

Published in Science Advances, the paper explores the ongoing “replication crisis” in which researchers have discovered that many findings in the fields of social sciences and medicine don’t hold up when other researchers try to repeat the experiments.

The paper reveals that findings from studies that cannot be verified when the experiments are repeated have a bigger influence over time. The unreliable research tends to be cited as if the results were true long after the publication failed to replicate.

“We also know that experts can predict well which papers will be replicated,” write the authors Marta Serra-Garcia, assistant professor of economics and strategy at the Rady School and Uri Gneezy, professor of behavioral economics also at the Rady School. “Given this prediction, we ask ‘Why are non-replicable papers accepted for publication in the first place?'”

Their possible answer is that review teams of academic journals face a trade-off. When the results are more “interesting,” they apply lower standards regarding their reproducibility.

Yearly Citation Count by Replicability

The average yearly citation count per year for studies that were not replicated (according to p value of the replication) in each replication study, and for those that were replicated. The light grey area shows the year(s) in which the original studies were published, and the dark line shows the year in which the replication study was published. Credit: UC San Diego

The link between interesting findings and nonreplicable research also can explain why it is cited at a much higher rate — the authors found that papers that successfully replicate are cited 153 times less than those that failed.

“Interesting or appealing findings are also covered more by media or shared on platforms like Twitter, generating a lot of attention, but that does not make them true,” Gneezy said.

Serra-Garcia and Gneezy analyzed data from three influential replication projects which tried to systematically replicate the findings in top psychology, economic and general science journals (Nature and Science). In psychology, only 39 percent of the 100 experiments successfully replicated. In economics, 61 percent of the 18 studies replicated as did 62 percent of the 21 studies published in Nature/Science.

With the findings from these three replication projects, the authors used Google Scholar to test whether papers that failed to replicate are cited significantly more often than those that were successfully replicated, both before and after the replication projects were published. The largest gap was in papers published in Nature/Science: non-replicable papers were cited 300 times more than replicable ones.

When the authors took into account several characteristics of the studies replicated — such as the number of authors, the rate of male authors, the details of the experiment (location, language, and online implementation) and the field in which the paper was published — the relationship between replicability and citations was unchanged.

They also show the impact of such citations grows over time. Yearly citation counts reveal a pronounced gap between papers that replicated and those that did not. On average, papers that failed to replicate are cited 16 times more per year. This gap remains even after the replication project is published.

“Remarkably, only 12 percent of post-replication citations of non-replicable findings acknowledge the replication failure,” the authors write.

The influence of an inaccurate paper published in a prestigious journal can have repercussions for decades. For example, the study Andrew Wakefield published in The Lancet in 1998 turned tens of thousands of parents around the world against the measles, mumps, and rubella vaccine because of an implied link between vaccinations and autism. The incorrect findings were retracted by The Lancet 12 years later, but the claims that autism is linked to vaccines continue.

The authors added that journals may feel pressure to publish interesting findings, and so do academics. For example, in promotion decisions, most academic institutions use citations as an important metric in the decision of whether to promote a faculty member.

This may be the source of the “replication crisis,” first discovered in the early 2010s.

“We hope our research encourages readers to be cautious if they read something that is interesting and appealing,” Serra-Garcia said. “Whenever researchers cite work that is more interesting or has been cited a lot, we hope they will check if replication data is available and what those findings suggest.”

Gneezy added, “We care about the field and producing quality research and we want to it to be true.”

Reference: “Nonreplicable publications are cited more than replicable ones” by Marta Serra-Garcia and Uri Gneezy, 21 May 2021, Science Advances.
DOI: 10.1126/sciadv.abd1705

1 Comment on "Science’s New Replication Crisis: Research That Is Less Likely to Be True Is Cited More"

  1. This is even a bigger problem than “research less likely to be true”, the reality is that the to pressure to find something significant or find exactly what someone payed scientists to find is the real problem. People are not honest specially when there is a lot of money involved, that includes scientists.

Leave a Reply to heinus Cancel reply

Email address is optional. If provided, your email will not be published or shared.