The Two Faces of Shyness: A New Study Analyzes Shyness in Children

Shy Young Girl

Researchers have conducted a study to better understand shyness in children, focusing on its behavioral, affective, and physiological components. Results indicated that temperamental shyness, a stable trait across development, may exist in a distinct group of children over time, while a larger subset may experience shyness as an emotional state in certain situations. The findings provide empirical support for longstanding theories on the differences between temperamental and state shyness, and have implications for understanding children’s social, psychological, and academic adjustment.

A new study explores shyness in children, finding that temperamental shyness may be a distinct trait in some, while others experience shyness as an emotional state in specific situations. The research offers insight into children’s social, psychological, and academic adjustment.

What is shyness? Research has shown that shyness is characterized by fear and nervousness in response to social novelty and/or social evaluation. Shyness can manifest on behavioral, affective, and physiological levels, but little is known about how these components cluster. Longstanding theories note that shyness may be conceptualized as a trait that is relatively stable across development, which is described as temperamental shyness. Shyness may also be conceptualized as an emotion that is felt in a certain social situation, which is described as state shyness.

To help better understand shyness in children, a new study released in the journal Child Development by researchers at McMaster University in Canada examined a child’s behavioral, affective, and physiological responses to a speech task. The findings showed that temperamental shyness may exist in a distinct group of children over time, while a larger subset of children may experience shyness as an emotional state in some situations.

“Our findings provide empirical support for the long-theorized idea that there may be a subset of temperamentally shy children who manifest heightened behavioral, affective, and physiological reactivity in response to a social stressor, as well as a subset of children who may experience only the affective component which may reflect state shyness,” as explained by Kristie Poole who conducted the study at McMaster University and is now a Banting Postdoctoral Fellow at Brock University. “This highlights the multiple components and developmental course of temperamental shyness and the features that distinguish temperamental and state shyness in middle to late childhood.”

The current study included 152 Canadian children (73 girls) aged 7-8 years and their primary caregivers. The children were born in a local hospital and were recruited from a child database at McMaster University containing birth records of infants whose parents consented to their infant’s inclusion. Ninety percent of participating caregivers were mothers and 10% were fathers. Children were primarily White (81.6%), followed by mixed race (9.9%), Asian (3.9%), Black (2.6%), and Latin American (2%). Children were primarily from middle to upper-socioeconomic-class families.

“The findings have implications for the conceptualization of shyness in that different types of shyness may differ in kind rather than degree.” — Kristie Poole

Children were fitted with an ambulatory electrocardiogram and completed activities with an experimenter in a room adjacent to their parent. During this time, parents completed online questionnaires related to the child’s temperament while monitoring their child on a muted closed-circuit monitor. Children prepared a two-minute speech about their last birthday and recited their speech in front of a video camera and mirror. They were told the speech would be videotaped for other children to watch later. This was designed to induce stress. The study team coded children’s avoidance/inhibition (i.e., behavior), children self-reported their nervousness (i.e., affect), and respiratory sinus arrhythmia (i.e. physiology) was measured.

For their time, families were given $20 gift cards and children received a Junior Scientist Certificate. At one- and two-years post evaluations, parents completed an online follow-up survey on their child’s temperament. They responded to statements such as “child acts shy around new people.” This examined how a child’s responses to the speech were related to their temperament across time. Parents were given a $10 gift card at each follow-up.

“The findings showed that approximately 10 percent of children in our study showed social stress reactivity to the speech on behavioral, affective, and physiological levels, and also had a pattern of relatively higher, stable parent-reported temperamental shyness across time, providing evidence that they may be characterized as temperamentally shy,” Poole continued. “A second subset of approximately 25 percent of children showed a pattern of social stress reactivity only on an affective level (i.e., self-reported feeling nervous), and did not show relatively high levels of parent-reported temperamental shyness, providing evidence that they may be characterized by state shyness. The findings have implications for the conceptualization of shyness in that different types of shyness may differ in kind rather than degree.”

The findings provide empirical evidence for long-standing ideas first articulated by the late Jerome Kagan several decades ago. He argued that temperamental shyness may exist as a distinct category for some children and the features that define this category are relatively stable across time and context. In addition to this subset of temperamentally shy children, researchers found that a larger subset of children may experience shyness as an emotional state in some situations. It is likely that the experience of state shyness in response to a speech task is a relatively common, normative experience for children at this age. For a smaller group of temperamentally shy children, however, being the center of attention may be stressful across time and various contexts. Since we know that not all children are alike and early temperamental shyness is a risk factor for internalizing-related problems, future work should examine the consequences of these findings for children’s social, psychological, and academic adjustment.

The authors acknowledge several limitations in their research. The study only measured behavioral, affective, and physiological components at one point in time, so they do not have the means to measure whether these components remain stable across development. The authors recommend that future research include more diverse samples of children as this study was primarily of White children from middle to upper socioeconomic status families making it difficult to generalize the findings.

Reference: “Latent profiles of children’s shyness: behavioral, affective, and physiological components” by Kristie L. Poole and Louis A. Schmidt, 25 April 2023, Child Development.
DOI: 10.1111/cdev.13920

This work was supported by a Canadian Institutes of Health Research Doctoral Award, an Elizabeth Munsterberg Koppitz Fellowship from the American Psychological Foundation, and funding from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (NSERC) and the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council (SSHRC).

3 Comments on "The Two Faces of Shyness: A New Study Analyzes Shyness in Children"

  1. Deborah Drake | May 7, 2023 at 10:01 pm | Reply

    Growing up in a very abusive household,being beat most every day,parents very verbally and alcoholic. I had severe shyness too along with terrible fear & and never wanting to speak. What that does to a child with severe shyness? Have you studied that also?

    • Hi Deborah
      I was thinking the exact same thing while reading this article. I am extremely shy for the reasons you mentioned.

  2. As someone who is shy, has a shy dad and I have 2 shy sons I find this article very interesting. Reading the above I’d say myself and my family members are temperamentally shy. This makes me wonder if there is a stronger genetic link to this type of shyness, and the state shyness could be more nurture based? I do feel like my shyness is with me for life, it’s annoying at times, but I do my best to transform and use it to help with my creative pursuits.

Leave a comment

Email address is optional. If provided, your email will not be published or shared.